Студопедия
Новини освіти і науки:
МАРК РЕГНЕРУС ДОСЛІДЖЕННЯ: Наскільки відрізняються діти, які виросли в одностатевих союзах


РЕЗОЛЮЦІЯ: Громадського обговорення навчальної програми статевого виховання


ЧОМУ ФОНД ОЛЕНИ ПІНЧУК І МОЗ УКРАЇНИ ПРОПАГУЮТЬ "СЕКСУАЛЬНІ УРОКИ"


ЕКЗИСТЕНЦІЙНО-ПСИХОЛОГІЧНІ ОСНОВИ ПОРУШЕННЯ СТАТЕВОЇ ІДЕНТИЧНОСТІ ПІДЛІТКІВ


Батьківський, громадянський рух в Україні закликає МОН зупинити тотальну сексуалізацію дітей і підлітків


Відкрите звернення Міністру освіти й науки України - Гриневич Лілії Михайлівні


Представництво українського жіноцтва в ООН: низький рівень культури спілкування в соціальних мережах


Гендерна антидискримінаційна експертиза може зробити нас моральними рабами


ЛІВИЙ МАРКСИЗМ У НОВИХ ПІДРУЧНИКАХ ДЛЯ ШКОЛЯРІВ


ВІДКРИТА ЗАЯВА на підтримку позиції Ганни Турчинової та права кожної людини на свободу думки, світогляду та вираження поглядів



Dominance versus subjection

According to Dorval (1990), in his study of same-sex friendly interaction, males tend to change subject more frequently than females. This difference may well be at the root of the conception that women chatter and talk too much, and may still trigger the same thinking in some males. In this way lowered estimation of women may arise. Incidentally, this attitude towards women as chatterers arguably arose from the idea that any female conversation was too much talking according to the patriarchal consideration of silence as a womanly virtue common to many cultures.

Men and women differ in their use of questions in conversations. For men, a question is usually a genuine request for information whereas for women it can often be a rhetorical means of being engaged into the conversation or acquiring attention from the interlocutors (Barnes, 1971). Therefore women use questions more frequently. In writing, however, both genders use rhetorical questions as literary devices.

One of the ways in which the communicative competence of men and women differs is in their use of minimal responses, i.e. paralinguistic features such as ‘mhm’ and ‘yeah’ associated with collaborative language use (Carli, 1990). Men, on the one hand, generally use yeah, mhm less frequently, and if they do, it is usually to show agreement in conversation. Female linguistic behavior expresses a desire to take turns in conversation with others.

Covert prestige generally assumes that non-standard is a low-prestige language. However, in certain groups, such as traditional working class neighborhoods, standard language may be considered undesirable in many contexts. This is because the working class dialect is a powerful in-group marker, and especially for non-mobile individuals, the use of non-standard varieties expresses a neighborhood pride, group, or age solidarity.

One can see different types of age-based language variation within the British population. They are: vernacular of a subgroup with the membership typically characterized by a specific age range, age-graded variation, and indications of linguistic change in progress. For example, people tend to use linguistic forms that were functioning when they reached adulthood. Examining the speech across several generations of a single family, sociolinguists found that the grandparents' generation would never or rarely merge (не различать) two vowel sounds caught and cot; their children's generation may on occasion, particularly in quick or informal speech merge them; while their grandchildren's generation would merge these two vowels uniformly. This is the basis of the apparent-time hypothesis where age-based variation is taken as an indication of a linguistic change in progress.

Geography differences cause the distribution of language or its constituent elements. There are two principal fields of study within the geography of language: the "geography of languages", which deals with the disposition of languages through history and space, and "linguistic geography", which deals with regional linguistic variations within languages.

To quote P. Trudgill, "linguistic geography has been geographical only in the sense that it has been concerned with the spatial distribution of linguistic phenomena”. In recent times greater emphasis has been laid upon explanation rather than description of the patterns of linguistic change. In these studies scholars have paid attention to the social use of languages, and to the dialect variety within languages in regard to social class or occupation. In England, linguistic geography has traditionally focused upon Rural English, rather than Urban English.

Men and women, on average, tend to use slightly different language styles. These differences tend to be quantitative rather than qualitative. That is to say, that women make more minimal responses than men. Robin Lakoff’s initial identification of a women's register stated that the women's style of language served to maintain an inferior role in society.

A later development of this argument was that gender differences in language reflect a power difference (Atkins’ dominance theory, 1980). However, more recently Deborah Tannen has proved that gender differences in language are more similar to 'cultural' differences. Comparing conversational goals, she argued that men have a report style, aiming to communicate a factual information, whereas women have a rapport (friendly) style, which is more concerned with building and maintaining relationships.

Communication styles are always a product of context, and as such, gender differences tend to be most pronounced in single-gender groups. One explanation of this is that people accommodate their style towards the language of the person they are interacting with. Thus, in a mixed-gender group, gender differences tend to be less pronounced. A similarly important observation is that this accommodation usually aims towards the language style, not the gender of the person.

Female tendencies toward self-disclosure, i.e. sharing their problems and experiences with others to offer sympathy (Dindia & Allen, 1992; Tannen, 1991) contrasts with male tendencies to non-self disclosure and professing advice or offering a solution when confronted with another’s problems.

Men tend to be more verbally aggressive in conversing (Labov, 1972), frequently using threats, profanities, yelling and name-calling (verbal abuse, especially as a substitute for reasoned argument in a dispute).Women, on the whole, deem this to disrupt the flow of conversation and not as a means of upholding one’s hierarchical status in the conversation. Where women swear, it is usually to demonstrate to others what normal behavior for them is.

This, in turn, suggests a dichotomy (противопоставление) between a male desire for conversational dominance – noted by Leet-Pellegrini (1980) with reference to male experts speaking more verbosely (многословно) than their female counterparts – and a female aspiration to group conversational participation. One way out of this is, according to Coates (1993), that males are afforded more attention in the context of the classroom and that this can lead to their gaining more attention in scientific and technical subjects, which in turn can lead to their achieving better success in those areas, ultimately leading to their having more power in a technocratic society.


Читайте також:

  1. Free Word-Groups Versus Phraseological Units Versus Words




Переглядів: 863

<== попередня сторінка | наступна сторінка ==>
 | 

Не знайшли потрібну інформацію? Скористайтесь пошуком google:

  

© studopedia.com.ua При використанні або копіюванні матеріалів пряме посилання на сайт обов'язкове.


Генерація сторінки за: 0.015 сек.